Table of Contents |
Short Report
| ||||||
Different forms of life satisfaction and their relation to affectivity | ||||||
Claudia Harzer1, Christian Ehrlich2 | ||||||
1Ph.D., Visiting Professor, Department of Psychology, University of Kassel, Germany.
2Ph.D., Senior Lecturer, Department of Business and Management, Oxford Brookes University, Oxford, UK. | ||||||
| ||||||
[HTML Abstract]
[PDF Full Text]
[Print This Article]
[Similar article in Pumed] [Similar article in Google Scholar] |
How to cite this article |
Harzer C, Ehrlich C. Different forms of life satisfaction and their relation to affectivity. Edorium J Psychol 2016;2:8–13. |
Abstract
|
Aims:
The present study is aimed at studying the research question whether high satisfaction levels achieved through either (1) the fact that one's quality of life is truly satisfactory (i.e., stabilized life satisfaction) or through (2) adapting a more favorable perception of one's quality of life by lowering standards (i.e., resigned life satisfaction) are associated with different levels of positive affect (PA) and negative affect (NA) when compared to each other but also when compared to dissatisfied individuals.
Case Report: A sample of 104 adults completed measures assessing stabilized and resigned life satisfaction, PA and NA, and global life satisfaction (i.e., no explicit consideration of differences in standards) in an online survey. Participants were not paid for participation but received feedback on results of the study if interest was expressed. Results: Stabilized life satisfaction and global life satisfaction showed highly similar correlation pattern with PA and NA. Resigned satisfaction was negatively associated with PA and positively correlated with NA. Stabilized and resigned satisfied individuals showed more favorable levels of PA and NA compared with dissatisfied individuals, but did not differ from each other with regard to their levels of PA and NA. Conclusion: Although showing a less favorable correlation pattern with PA and NA, adopting a resigned form of satisfaction (i.e., taking a more favorable view of one's quality of life by lowering individual standards) seemed to prevent individuals from experiencing the same high levels of NA like dissatisfied ones. This has not been studied in detail so far, and therefore, the present paper opens a further area of research within the context of life satisfaction. | |
Keywords:
Affectivity, Individual standards, Life satisfaction, Resigned satisfaction, Stabilized satisfaction
|
Introduction
| ||||||
Life satisfaction measures are widely accepted as an indicator for people's quality of life [1]. However, they tend to be negatively skewed with average scores typically way above the neutral point of the scale used [2] [3] Acknowledging the plethora of research that identified a number of biases impacting on satisfaction scores [4], this paper argues that one important cognitive process, which leads to relatively high satisfaction scores, has, so far, been mainly overlooked. That is, people at times lower their standards with which they judge the quality of their life [5] [6] to perceive life in a more favorable way. Thus, one contributing factor to high satisfaction scores can be due to the fact that a proportion of individuals start to cognitively re-evaluate their life when their life does not meet their expectations in order to reduce cognitive dissonance, and subsequently perceive life in a more favorable way [7]. Against this backdrop, the question arises whether above average levels of satisfaction, either because (1) life is truly satisfactory or (2) because standards have been lowered, are associated differently with other important indicators of people's quality of life such as affective subjective well-being (SWB; i.e., positive and negative affect). Different forms of satisfaction In the light of this, we argue that individual standards can be on different levels and that the fulfillment of these different standards leads to different forms of satisfaction. Within the scope of the present paper, we distinguish between a stabilized and a resigned form of satisfaction [10][11]. Stabilized satisfaction is characterized by having maintained the original, genuine standards. Resigned satisfaction is "positively inflated" as it is characterized by lowered standards to make life appear more positive. Both forms of satisfaction are characterized by a fulfillment of individual standards as opposed to dissatisfied individuals who feel that life does not meet their standards. Aims and hypotheses | ||||||
Materials and Methods
| ||||||
Participants Instruments Affective SWB was measured using the Mood Survey [14], which includes 16 adjectives such as happy, depressed, vigorous, and unmotivated to measure positive affect (PA) and negative affect (NA). The adjectives have to be answered with respect to how often those affects have been experienced during the last couple of months ranging from 1 = not at all to 7 = very frequently. The mood survey showed satisfactory reliability and validity [14] [15]. The PA and the NA scales had internal consistencies of α = 0.95 (α = 0.96 for females, α = 0.91 for males) and α = 0.93 (α = 0.92 for females, α = 0.93 for males) in the present sample, respectively. Global life satisfaction (i.e., cognitive SWB) was measured with the satisfaction with life scale (SWLS; [1]), which is a widely used five-item measure of life satisfaction utilizing a seven-point answer format (from 1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree). The SWLS does not consider any differences in standards [1]. The German version of the SWLS administered in the present study, showed satisfactory reliability and validity [16] [17] [18][19]. It revealed high internal consistency in the present sample (α = 0.87; α = 0.88 for females, α = 0.85 for males). Procedure | ||||||
Results | ||||||
Descriptive analyses of the measures Overall, stabilized life satisfaction and life satisfaction showed highly similar correlation pattern with positive and negative affect. Resigned satisfaction was negatively associated with PA and positively correlated with NA indicating that resigned satisfaction included aspects of dissatisfaction. Forms of life satisfaction and affect ANCOVAs indicated that group membership had an effect on PA (F [2, 104] = 6.04, p < 0.05, partial η2 = 0.111), and on NA (F [2, 104] = 6.36, p < 0.01, partial η2 = 0.116). Post hoc tests revealed no significant differences between resigned and stabilized satisfied individuals in PA or NA. However, both resigned and stabilized satisfied individuals showed significant lower levels of NA, and significant higher levels of PA compared with dissatisfied individuals (Figure 1). Thus, the findings are in support of hypotheses 2, 3 and 4. Hypothesis 1 is rejected. | ||||||
| ||||||
| ||||||
| ||||||
| ||||||
Discussion
| ||||||
The findings of this study highlight the importance of individual standards as a constituting element of life satisfaction. More precisely, the findings suggest that lowering standards is a beneficial way of keeping satisfaction levels moderately high with no significant differences in affective SWB when compared with stabilized individuals. Moreover, the benefits with regards to PA and NA compared with dissatisfied individuals are significant. With regard to the practical implications of these findings, it seems important to remember that the relatively high levels of satisfaction, through adapting a resigned form of satisfaction, come about through changes within the individual and are therefore not a true reflection of the (objective) quality of their life as such. This is in line with research that shows that life satisfaction levels are often unrelated to objective indicators of people's lives [4]. Assuming that the ability to perceive one's life in a more favorable way is limited and the notion that important life changes can lead to permanent drops in happiness [20], it seems important to monitor the proportion as well as the level of resignation within a certain population. Additionally, there is evidence that lowering one's expectation can be a successful strategy to avoid disappointment and therefore prevent the experience of low negative affect [21]. Further research is, therefore, needed to study when a change of standards is adaptive but equally at which point adapting a resigned form of satisfaction is a detrimental coping strategy. This latter is especially important in the light of correlational results presented here that indicated the resigned satisfaction goes along with lower levels of positive affect and higher levels of negative affect. The findings have to be treated with care because of the preliminary nature of the study. Particularly, because our measure for resigned and stabilized life satisfaction is simply an adapted version of items on job satisfaction and has only been tested rudimentary. This might in part be responsible for the insignificant differences in positive and negative affect between stabilized and resigned satisfaction although they were expected. Equally, our findings are based on a relatively small sample. Furthermore, sample was disproportionally highly educated and therefore, generalizability may be limited. Future research might develop a more advanced scale, which measures the level of satisfaction independently from the level of resignation which would overcome the currently given need to measure resigned satisfaction with items that include two contents (satisfaction levels and level of resignation), which does not fully comply with the rules for item construction. Furthermore, larger and more representative samples need to be studied to ensure statistical power and generalizability of results. | ||||||
Conclusion
| ||||||
When studying life satisfaction it seems to be important to take a closer look at the level of standards. Scores from life satisfaction measures tend to be negatively skewed with average scores typically way above the neutral point of the scale used. This might be in part be explained by processes within individuals who cognitively re-evaluate their life to reduce cognitive dissonance when it does not meet their expectations, and subsequently perceive it in a more favorable way. Hence, the present paper opens a further area of research within the context of life satisfaction. | ||||||
Acknowledgements
| ||||||
The preparation of this paper has been facilitated by a research grant of the Swiss National Science Foundation (SNSF; PBZHP1_147249) awarded to Claudia Harzer. | ||||||
References
| ||||||
|
[HTML Abstract]
[PDF Full Text]
|
Author Contributions:
Claudia Harzer – Substantial contributions to conception and design, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting the article, Revising it critically for important intellectual content, Final approval of the version to be published Christian Ehrlich – Analysis and interpretation of data, Revising it critically for important intellectual content, Final approval of the version to be published |
Guarantor of submission
The corresponding author is the guarantor of submission. |
Source of support
None |
Conflict of interest
Authors declare no conflict of interest. |
Copyright
© 2016 Claudia Harzer et al. This article is distributed under the terms of Creative Commons Attribution License which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided the original author(s) and original publisher are properly credited. Please see the copyright policy on the journal website for more information. |
|